Publishers of technology books, eBooks, and videos for creative people

Home > Articles

This chapter is from the book

A Closer Look

Politically insensitive jokes about vision deficiencies are exceedingly common in the publishing industry—normally with respect to visual (if not mental) impairments allegedly suffered by clients who offer certain questionable opinions about color.

Even the best of us can suffer this indignity. Once when I supervised a pressrun, the pressmen, offended by some of my decisions the previous time around, announced that the only reason I was being permitted to do it again was that Stevie Wonder had turned the job down and Ray Charles was out of town.

Calling other people blind is a frustrated reaction to what everybody knows: we all see color differently and therefore we all have different, sometimes radically different, ideas of what looks good.

One of the most irritating challenges we face today is determining what constitutes a visual match. So, without further ado, you are hereby assigned to make such a decision. In Figure 3.12, #1 is the original. Which of the other four do you think is the closest match to it? Which the worst?

53_fig3_12.jpg

Figure 3.12 The question is, supposing that #1 is the reference image, which of the others is the closest— and which is the worst— visual match to it?

This question isn't as weird as you might think, provided you visualize #1 as being held in your left hand while the rest of the book is held in your right. Say #1 is found in an annual report that uses much whiter paper than this book does, for example. In that case, we wouldn't be able to match it exactly. Nevertheless, we'd try to get close—and choosing the best way to do so is not as easy a call as this one appears to be.

I empaneled a jury of 12 to come up with the answer here and in several other sets of images. You probably won't agree with their verdicts, particularly (speaking of politically insensitive commentary) if you are female.

Justice may be blind, but this jury pushed the envelope. I decided to find out how bad it would be if clients were actually as visually impaired as many retouchers and printing firms claim. To that end, I empaneled a jury of the color-blind. And color blindness is almost exclusively a male phenomenon.

The Protanope-a-Dope Strategy

Color blindness is caused by a sex-related recessive gene. Men have only one of these genes, and the wrong one turns up around eight percent of the time. Women have two. If either one is normal, the woman has no problem herself, although if the other is bad, half of her male children will be color-blind. For the same reason, men are vastly more likely to be afflicted by the disastrous blood illness hemophilia. According to my wife, sex-related recessive genes also exist for dishonesty, shallowness, poor listening skills, and inability to keep a kitchen clean, although the scientific evidence to support her position is as yet inconclusive.

The term color-blind is misleading, because those afflicted have no problem seeing many colors. Plus, some individuals have more of a deficiency than others.

The overwhelming majority of the color-blind, including all members of my jury, are said to suffer from red-green color blindness. This, too, is a misleading term. Magenta-green is a more accurate description. I showed the jury the famous Kodak picture of Figure 3.13 and asked what color the woman's hat was. All but one responded "Brilliant red."

54_fig3_13.jpg

Figure 3.13 The common term for the affliction is the inaccurate "red-green color blindness." A group of such individuals nevertheless had no difficulty saying what color this woman's hat is.

There are at least two categories (some say four) of magenta-green deficiencies. Part of my testing was a protanope-and-deuteronope-adope strategy aimed at finding which category each juror belonged to. It turned out that the class of deficiency played almost no role in the men's evaluations of the images.

If individuals with normal vision were evaluating Figure 3.12, I'd expect a unanimous vote for #3 as best match to #1, and #5 as worst match. I would also expect, but not guarantee, a decision that #2 is closer to #1 than #4 is. The color-blind jury, of course, felt very differently.

This was one of a series of similar comparisons, all produced in LAB. The black numbers were burned into each image so that they could conveniently be viewed on screen.

In each case, I created one alternative by horsing around with the L channel, while doing nothing to the AB. That version (#3 in this set) therefore matched the original for color but not for detail. If you're normally sighted, that's probably your favorite, because the color in all three alternatives is so abysmal. But we can't pretend that it actually matches.

There was always a second version (#4 here) where I flattened the A curve in just the same way that we've steepened it in every example so far. That is, the center point stayed the same, but instead of rotating the curve counterclockwise around it, I rotated it clockwise. This savagely reduced saturation along the magenta-green axis.

In a third version (#2 here) I flattened the A only half as much, but I also modestly flattened the yellow-blue B channel. And there was always a trick image, #5 here, that was meant to prove a point and about which I asked specific questions. The trick in Figure 3.12 is that #5 is identical to #4 except that the A has been inverted; that is, everything that tended toward magenta now tends toward green and vice versa.

The jury always hated the version that toned down the yellow-blue channel. If the colors of the image were relatively subdued, as in this river shot, the jurors tended to like the version that matched for detail but washed out the magenta-green distinction. In more brilliant shots, they started to abandon the version with the unchanged L in favor of the one with true colors. The only one in which the wrong-detail picture decisively beat the magenta-green washout was this Kodak image, where the hat is apparently so red that even the color-blind take notice.

Since Figure 3.12 doesn't feature many bright colors, only three jurors voted for #3 as the best. Yet only three went for the normal winner, #4. The other six rated it an unusual kind of tie.

I asked for a description of the differences between #4 and #5. Half the jury replied that the two are identical. Five people saw significant differences, but only one offered a description reasonably similar to what a normally sighted person might say. The others made comments like "There is a saturation issue in the trees."

Those who thought there was something seriously messed up about #5, even without knowing what it was, naturally voted it as the worst. The other six did the conventional thing for the color-blind, choosing #2 as worst match.

Those who saw #4 and #5 as identical all voted the two of them as best match to #1. This makes sense. These people can't see the magenta-green component at all, so they completely ignored the flat-looking colors of #4. They only saw that #4 and #5 match the detailing of #1 and that #3 doesn't.

One of the most valuable attributes anyone working in the graphic arts can develop is the ability to predict how clients may react to certain images. Given what you know now about this color-blind jury, how do you think it felt about the Halloween image of Figure 3.14? Once again, image #1 is the reference version. You are supposed to say which of the other four matches it the best, and which the worst.

55_fig3_14.jpg

Figure 3.14 Another challenge to the color-blind jury: assuming that #1 is correct, which of the other four is the closest visual match to it, and which the worst?

It Can't Be Put into Words

The variations here follow the same lines as before, with the numbers shuffled. Version #2 corresponds to #4 in Figure 3.12: the magenta-green component is sharply reduced by a severe flattening of the A. Figure 3.14's #3 is the equivalent of Figure 3.12's #3: the color is the same, but there's a slight change in the L, causing a variance in detail. The overall reduction in all color that was #2 in Figure 3.12 is #5 in Figure 3.14. #4 is the trick image, a specialized treatment of the A channel in which darker magentas, such as the man's jacket, were left alone, but softer colors, such as his face, were grayed out. And I asked for specific comments comparing #4 and #5 of Figure 3.14.

With a single exception, the jury made its normal choice for the best, splitting between #2 and #3. Version 5, with its diminution of the yellow-blue component that the color-blind see well, was, as usual, voted worst, but there were also several votes for #4 as worst, presumably from those who could see enough of the magenta component to have a serious issue with the man's face.

The group had a hard time evaluating the man's jacket in #4 and #5. Let me paraphrase some of their comments:

  • One is lighter.
  • One is more saturated.
  • One is punchier.
  • One is browner.
  • There's no difference at all. As for the clown's costume, most of the jury correctly said that it was more yellow in #4. But, strangely, four jurors said that in #5, it had turned green.

Accordingly, if you're thinking of designing graphics with the color-blind in mind, you have to accept that there's a lot of variation in the group. These jury findings suggest that the best way to predict how a color-blind person would react to a picture is to convert it to LAB and apply the curve shown in Figure 3.15— the same curve that produced version #4 of the river scene and #2 of the Halloween image. Keeping that in mind, how do you think the jury voted in the simplest of all these sets, the vividly colored Figure 3.16?

56_fig3_15.jpg

Figure 3.15 The quickest way to approximate how a color-blind person might see an image is to apply this curve to the A channel.

hockey1.jpg

Figure 3.16 This image features more vivid colors than those found in Figures 3.12 and 3.14. How do you think the color-blind jury voted?

The variant of #1 that uses this curve is #2, and that's the version that the color-blind think is a closer match than #5, which seems so obviously closest to the rest of us that we don't notice that its darkness doesn't quite match.

The jury always disliked anything that hurt the B channel, like #3 here, which is analogous to #2 in Figure 3.12 and #5 in Figure 3.14. I offered them a choice of poisons by generating #4, in which everything red has been wiped out (damaging the B channel as well as the A), while pure blues and yellows were left alone. The vote for worst match to #1 was a dead heat between these two versions.

Using Figure 3.15's curve is a good deal more accurate than just assuming that people are "red-green color-blind," or that they see a black and white world, but it has severe limitations, in that nobody has ever been both color-blind and normally sighted in the same lifetime. Thus, no eyewitnesses can be called.

The conventional wisdom is that those who are deprived of one sense compensate by becoming more acutely sensitive in the others. Do we know how a symphony of Beethoven sounds to a blind person? Or whether deaf people are able to see things in Renoir that the rest of us cannot?

There was only one unanimous response to any of the questions. In an ocean shot, the water was not particularly vividly colored. I asked in which version it was the least blue. The question was difficult enough to stump some normally sighted persons. Every member of the jury nevertheless got it right. They appear to respond to blue at least as well as the rest of us do. Is it possible that they actually see it better?

Nobody has any trouble detecting that the clown's costume is less yellow in #5 than in any of the other variants in Figure 3.14. But what do you make of the strong minority sentiment that it's gone green? I myself don't see greenness, yet by measuring the numbers that make up the jacket, one can construct the argument that it is in fact more green. Can the color-blind be picking up a distinction that's too subtle for the rest of us? We can see colors that aren't even on their map, but is it not possible that the converse may also be true—that the green they are reporting is simply a color that's out of our gamut?

In that Kodak picture of Figure 3.13, are the color-blind just seeing a duller-colored fleshtone, or do they detect more yellowness as well? Do they see her hair as a brilliant yellow, rather than the mousy eyesore the rest of us perceive?

And What If God Is Color-Blind?

In 1994, in the first edition of Professional Photoshop, I showed examples of the work of a colorblind person who had been trained to correct images, by the numbers perforce.

That sideshow received more attention from readers than it actually deserved. But it was a measure of how we are fascinated by the way other people perceive things.

Some of us are fortunate enough to create images that need please only ourselves. But most professionals have to please a client, or a reader, or an art director, or somebody else whose preferences—and perhaps, whose ability to see certain colors at all—is in question. And everyone who has been there has found out the hard way about the consequences of these perceptual differences.

Granted, having a color-blind person evaluate your work is tough. If your client tells you that #2, #4, and #2 are the best matches to #1 in the river, Halloween, and hockey images, respectively, it can be a little disconcerting. Yet this is merely an extreme case of what happens everyday in the real world. As people age, their corneas get yellower and they lose perception in certain shades. Also, many drugs, Viagra being a notorious example, affect color perception.

The questions listed just above the last subhead therefore are not just an academic matter. If people see color differently than we do, we can hardly blame them for acting on it. And who knows, perhaps they're right.

Having our own opinion about these matters is both natural and desirable, provided we don't take it too seriously. And, given the differences in human perception, we have to realize that the client is king. After all, in the land of the color-blind, the spectrophotometer isn't even the scullery maid.

At high noon in hell, when viewing conditions are the best, punishment is meted out to the unholy. Every color management consultant who has overhyped his technology or overrelied on the measurements of an artificial measuring instrument, is forced, day after day for all eternity, to argue and demonstrate the accuracy of his profiles—to a jury of the color-blind.

Perhaps it is all a matter of divine humor and spite. How do we know what is normal, and what is defective? Perhaps God Himself sees the clown's jacket as green, and has difficulty with magentas. Perhaps our own ability to distinguish these magentas and greens is a handicap, a cosmic joke, and not reality at all.

Fortunately, there's no need to decide this issue, not that it could ever be decided. We must merely concede that there is not necessarily a correct answer to the question of which version is the best match, just as there is not necessarily a correct answer as to which looks the best.

Magenta, green, yellow, blue. The wavelengths of light intrude upon us, enter our consciousness, trick us into thinking we see an absolute, laughing at us the whole while, knowing that for all the certainty we feel in our own perceptions, each of us is just a bit color-blind.

Peachpit Promotional Mailings & Special Offers

I would like to receive exclusive offers and hear about products from Peachpit and its family of brands. I can unsubscribe at any time.

Overview


Pearson Education, Inc., 221 River Street, Hoboken, New Jersey 07030, (Pearson) presents this site to provide information about Peachpit products and services that can be purchased through this site.

This privacy notice provides an overview of our commitment to privacy and describes how we collect, protect, use and share personal information collected through this site. Please note that other Pearson websites and online products and services have their own separate privacy policies.

Collection and Use of Information


To conduct business and deliver products and services, Pearson collects and uses personal information in several ways in connection with this site, including:

Questions and Inquiries

For inquiries and questions, we collect the inquiry or question, together with name, contact details (email address, phone number and mailing address) and any other additional information voluntarily submitted to us through a Contact Us form or an email. We use this information to address the inquiry and respond to the question.

Online Store

For orders and purchases placed through our online store on this site, we collect order details, name, institution name and address (if applicable), email address, phone number, shipping and billing addresses, credit/debit card information, shipping options and any instructions. We use this information to complete transactions, fulfill orders, communicate with individuals placing orders or visiting the online store, and for related purposes.

Surveys

Pearson may offer opportunities to provide feedback or participate in surveys, including surveys evaluating Pearson products, services or sites. Participation is voluntary. Pearson collects information requested in the survey questions and uses the information to evaluate, support, maintain and improve products, services or sites; develop new products and services; conduct educational research; and for other purposes specified in the survey.

Contests and Drawings

Occasionally, we may sponsor a contest or drawing. Participation is optional. Pearson collects name, contact information and other information specified on the entry form for the contest or drawing to conduct the contest or drawing. Pearson may collect additional personal information from the winners of a contest or drawing in order to award the prize and for tax reporting purposes, as required by law.

Newsletters

If you have elected to receive email newsletters or promotional mailings and special offers but want to unsubscribe, simply email ask@peachpit.com.

Service Announcements

On rare occasions it is necessary to send out a strictly service related announcement. For instance, if our service is temporarily suspended for maintenance we might send users an email. Generally, users may not opt-out of these communications, though they can deactivate their account information. However, these communications are not promotional in nature.

Customer Service

We communicate with users on a regular basis to provide requested services and in regard to issues relating to their account we reply via email or phone in accordance with the users' wishes when a user submits their information through our Contact Us form.

Other Collection and Use of Information


Application and System Logs

Pearson automatically collects log data to help ensure the delivery, availability and security of this site. Log data may include technical information about how a user or visitor connected to this site, such as browser type, type of computer/device, operating system, internet service provider and IP address. We use this information for support purposes and to monitor the health of the site, identify problems, improve service, detect unauthorized access and fraudulent activity, prevent and respond to security incidents and appropriately scale computing resources.

Web Analytics

Pearson may use third party web trend analytical services, including Google Analytics, to collect visitor information, such as IP addresses, browser types, referring pages, pages visited and time spent on a particular site. While these analytical services collect and report information on an anonymous basis, they may use cookies to gather web trend information. The information gathered may enable Pearson (but not the third party web trend services) to link information with application and system log data. Pearson uses this information for system administration and to identify problems, improve service, detect unauthorized access and fraudulent activity, prevent and respond to security incidents, appropriately scale computing resources and otherwise support and deliver this site and its services.

Cookies and Related Technologies

This site uses cookies and similar technologies to personalize content, measure traffic patterns, control security, track use and access of information on this site, and provide interest-based messages and advertising. Users can manage and block the use of cookies through their browser. Disabling or blocking certain cookies may limit the functionality of this site.

Do Not Track

This site currently does not respond to Do Not Track signals.

Security


Pearson uses appropriate physical, administrative and technical security measures to protect personal information from unauthorized access, use and disclosure.

Children


This site is not directed to children under the age of 13.

Marketing


Pearson may send or direct marketing communications to users, provided that

  • Pearson will not use personal information collected or processed as a K-12 school service provider for the purpose of directed or targeted advertising.
  • Such marketing is consistent with applicable law and Pearson's legal obligations.
  • Pearson will not knowingly direct or send marketing communications to an individual who has expressed a preference not to receive marketing.
  • Where required by applicable law, express or implied consent to marketing exists and has not been withdrawn.

Pearson may provide personal information to a third party service provider on a restricted basis to provide marketing solely on behalf of Pearson or an affiliate or customer for whom Pearson is a service provider. Marketing preferences may be changed at any time.

Correcting/Updating Personal Information


If a user's personally identifiable information changes (such as your postal address or email address), we provide a way to correct or update that user's personal data provided to us. This can be done on the Account page. If a user no longer desires our service and desires to delete his or her account, please contact us at customer-service@informit.com and we will process the deletion of a user's account.

Choice/Opt-out


Users can always make an informed choice as to whether they should proceed with certain services offered by Adobe Press. If you choose to remove yourself from our mailing list(s) simply visit the following page and uncheck any communication you no longer want to receive: www.peachpit.com/u.aspx.

Sale of Personal Information


Pearson does not rent or sell personal information in exchange for any payment of money.

While Pearson does not sell personal information, as defined in Nevada law, Nevada residents may email a request for no sale of their personal information to NevadaDesignatedRequest@pearson.com.

Supplemental Privacy Statement for California Residents


California residents should read our Supplemental privacy statement for California residents in conjunction with this Privacy Notice. The Supplemental privacy statement for California residents explains Pearson's commitment to comply with California law and applies to personal information of California residents collected in connection with this site and the Services.

Sharing and Disclosure


Pearson may disclose personal information, as follows:

  • As required by law.
  • With the consent of the individual (or their parent, if the individual is a minor)
  • In response to a subpoena, court order or legal process, to the extent permitted or required by law
  • To protect the security and safety of individuals, data, assets and systems, consistent with applicable law
  • In connection the sale, joint venture or other transfer of some or all of its company or assets, subject to the provisions of this Privacy Notice
  • To investigate or address actual or suspected fraud or other illegal activities
  • To exercise its legal rights, including enforcement of the Terms of Use for this site or another contract
  • To affiliated Pearson companies and other companies and organizations who perform work for Pearson and are obligated to protect the privacy of personal information consistent with this Privacy Notice
  • To a school, organization, company or government agency, where Pearson collects or processes the personal information in a school setting or on behalf of such organization, company or government agency.

Links


This web site contains links to other sites. Please be aware that we are not responsible for the privacy practices of such other sites. We encourage our users to be aware when they leave our site and to read the privacy statements of each and every web site that collects Personal Information. This privacy statement applies solely to information collected by this web site.

Requests and Contact


Please contact us about this Privacy Notice or if you have any requests or questions relating to the privacy of your personal information.

Changes to this Privacy Notice


We may revise this Privacy Notice through an updated posting. We will identify the effective date of the revision in the posting. Often, updates are made to provide greater clarity or to comply with changes in regulatory requirements. If the updates involve material changes to the collection, protection, use or disclosure of Personal Information, Pearson will provide notice of the change through a conspicuous notice on this site or other appropriate way. Continued use of the site after the effective date of a posted revision evidences acceptance. Please contact us if you have questions or concerns about the Privacy Notice or any objection to any revisions.

Last Update: November 17, 2020